Confusion about science and technology
post by NancyLebovitz · 2013-10-23T12:27:55.524Z · LW · GW · Legacy · 36 commentsContents
36 comments
Science is not particularly reliable.
And yet, we have remarkable technology, and can do medical marvels.
My tentative theory is that there's a lot of knowledge that's less formal than science in engineering, manufacturing, and the practice of medicine which makes it possible to get work done, and some fairly effective methods of filtering information that comes from science.
36 comments
Comments sorted by top scores.
comment by gattsuru · 2013-10-23T16:29:35.802Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
My tentative theory is that there's a lot of knowledge that's less formal than science in engineering, manufacturing, and the practice of medicine which makes it possible to get work done, and some fairly effective methods of filtering information that comes from science.
Honestly? Not really.
There are some filters, true. If you work in biochem, for example, you will end up trying to follow someone's instructions in a lab setting, and you eventually learn better than to read certain open access publishers and that certain numbers are red flags. I've personally tried to use someone's image processing code from a research paper, and found that it wouldn't even compile in the version of MatLab they claimed to use, and wouldn't have worked even if it did compile. That ended up being a simple and correctable error, but there are far less forgivable horror stories in the machine learning field. The Schon scandal was discovered when folk tried to replicate things in the lab, and it just didn't work. There have been some efforts to computationally model natural data sets versus fraudulent ones, although I've not yet seen any serious implementation.
There are some structural issues that make the fields of experimental psychology and experimental sociology a little more prone to unreliability and fraud, but they're not as unique to those fields as we'd like. The biggest thing is the time cost of replicating experiments and natural variance in test subjects, and that's an issue in practical medicinal science and even engineering, too.
We didn't find out about Stapel's fraud for years, even though most of his works required no greater research tools than bored undergrads. Vioxx had a test case of eighty million people, which it didn't really need given that the heart disease risks showed up in vastly smaller groups. It took a decade and a half for the fraud in the Wakefield study to finally be retracted. And it's impossible to measure the degree of fraud or error we don't catch.
The practical difference is that we've gotten the larger tests cases, often at the cost of significant amounts of sweat and blood. Sometimes for much lesser sins than fraud or irreproducibility. Where we didn't think through every possible mechanical flaw or didn't test the materials completely enough, bridges collapsed with people on them. Where a simple math error occurs, over a hundred million USD goes exactly the wrong way. And we learn.
The only meaningful filter is reality. Peer review will forgive a great many errors. Physics does not.
comment by Neph · 2013-10-24T05:53:19.723Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
I know this may come off as a "no true scotsman" argument, but this is a bit different- bear with me. consider christianity (yes, I'm bringing religion into this, sort of...) in the beginning, we have a single leader preaching a set of morals that is (arguably) correct from a utilitarian standpoint, and calling all who follow that set "christians" by so doing, he created what Influence: Science and Practice would call "the trappings of morality" ...so basically, fast-forward a few hundered years, and we have people who think they can do whatever they like and it'll be morally right, so long as they wear a cross doing it. parallel to the current situation: we set up science- a set of rules that will always result in truth, if followed. by so doing, we created the trappings of right-ness. fast forward to now, and we have a bunch of people who think they can decide whatever they want, and it'll be right, so long as they wear a labcoat while doing it. understand, that's a bit of metaphor, in truth, these "scientists" (scoff) simply learned the rules of science by rote without really understanding what they mean. to them, reproducible results is just something nice to have as part of the ritual of science, instead of something completely necessary to get the right answer
...all of this stuff I said, by the way, is said in one of the core sequences, but I'm not sure which. I may reply to myself later with the link to the sequence in question.
Replies from: NancyLebovitz↑ comment by NancyLebovitz · 2013-10-25T13:59:51.271Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
I don't know whether it's what you're thinking about, but I file that sort of thing under Goodhart's Law-- any measurement which is used to guide policy will become corrupted.
comment by Anatoly_Vorobey · 2013-10-23T16:58:45.793Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
Science is amazingly reliable compared to any other method of generating knowledge about the world.
It's misleading to talk about science in general in this context, as different sciences have different amount of "kicking back" going on. The more the world kicks back, and the faster it happens, the easier it is for scientists to falsify & iterate. Technology is based on sciences with some of the most testable hypotheses. This doesn't mean (necessarily) that scientific articles are more reliable in those areas, but the unreliable ones are detected faster.
We happen to live in a universe where physical interactions are highly local, and forces generally fall off very quickly with distance. On the typical scales we build things at, the physical laws are also very close to deterministic and can be modeled with high accuracy by simplified models with few entities (e.g. looking at a body as a point particle with mass at its center of mass). There's an anthropic argument about why this should be so somewhere in Feynman, I forget where. Taken all together, this allows very high reproducibility, which enables rapid development of technology by iterated experiments.
Indeed, engineering is distinct from science and contains vast lore all of its own that's not reducible to basic science.
I feel like I've just written a bunch of trivialities.
↑ comment by NancyLebovitz · 2013-10-23T19:19:51.878Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
Indeed, engineering is distinct from science and contains vast lore all of its own that's not reducible to basic science.
To my mind, that's the interesting one. Does the lore ever get fed back into science?
Replies from: moridinamael↑ comment by moridinamael · 2013-10-24T00:51:36.936Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
I would say there is not a sharp dividing line. There is engineering practice, there is research and development into incremental modifications and improvements of existing engineering practice having varying degrees of novelty, and way at the other end of the spectrum is pure research into the mass of neutrinos and whatnot. In between there is an infinite range of degrees.
In some sense engineers are always doing "science." Pilot projects and prototypes are a common way of experimentally demonstrating the feasibility of a new engineering design or process. One might say that this is "science" but not "Science." Some seem to feel that it isn't Science without peer review. I've been part of the peer review process numerous times from both sides of the table and it's nothing like what you would think if you gleaned your impression of peer review from reading about it on lesswrong. In short, the process barely serves to filter out the obviously wrong.
Replies from: jamesf, passive_fist↑ comment by jamesf · 2013-10-26T07:37:04.071Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
The market provides a continuous and generally valid test of engineering principles. I think it's more scientific than peer review, in the most meaningful sense of the word "science".
Replies from: twanvl↑ comment by twanvl · 2013-10-29T15:26:04.377Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
Not all engineering is about developing products to sell to consumers. Engineers also design bridges and rockets. I don't think these are subject to the open marker in any meaningful sense.
Replies from: Lumifer↑ comment by Lumifer · 2013-10-29T17:48:44.316Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
Engineers also design bridges and rockets. I don't think these are subject to the open marker in any meaningful sense.
Rockets (until recently) had only one buyer, true, but bridges are certainly subject to the open market. When, say, a government entity wants to build a bridge it writes down the specs and invites people to submit designs and expected costs -- there's your open market.
↑ comment by passive_fist · 2013-10-26T08:22:49.204Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
Some of the best science has come out of the engineering industry, actually, and this is widely recognized (look at how many nobel prizes in physics were awarded to people who did work in the electronics industry; the 1956 prize for transistors stands out in particular).
In industry the stakes are higher and there is a higher penalty for being wrong about the world. This drives a lot of good science.
comment by lmm · 2013-10-23T13:20:08.548Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
Science is a funnel of filters; there's no single point - not publishing, not peer review, not cite counts - that reliably distingushes true hypotheses from false ones. But taken together, it works.
Have you tried to test your idea? Are engineers better than scientists at predicting which results will turn out to be true?
Replies from: Viliam_Bur, Vaniver, ChristianKl↑ comment by Viliam_Bur · 2013-10-23T14:38:07.313Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
Are engineers better than scientists at predicting which results will turn out to be true?
Scientists have to come with new ideas, engineers are allowed (even encouraged) to repeat already tried ideas. So I would expect engineers to get much better predictions.
Replies from: lmm↑ comment by lmm · 2013-10-23T16:20:22.625Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
Well sure, I'd expect the predictions an engineer makes professionally to be more accurate than those a scientist does. I meant if we tested them on the same set of predictions (e.g. recent experimental results which didn't yet have a replication attempt)
↑ comment by Vaniver · 2013-10-23T18:42:51.632Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
Have you tried to test your idea? Are engineers better than scientists at predicting which results will turn out to be true?
A core difference between scientists and engineers is that engineers only need one working prototype to validate a design, and scientists only need one strong counterexample to invalidate a hypothesis.
(Note that some fields where practitioners call themselves scientists but operate much more like engineers, with 'studies' as prototypes.)
↑ comment by ChristianKl · 2013-10-23T17:24:58.905Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
Science is a funnel of filters; there's no single point - not publishing, not peer review, not cite counts - that reliably distingushes true hypotheses from false ones. But taken together, it works.
How do you know that it works? How would the world look like if engineers got stuff working by tinkering instead of basing their work on the work of scientists?
Replies from: Lumifer↑ comment by Lumifer · 2013-10-23T17:38:55.729Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
How do you know that it works?
Because you're typing these words looking at a screen and the words are magically transported all around the world to appear at screens of other people...
How would the world look like
Pretty medieval, I think. I don't see why engineers would discover and develop electricity, to start with, never mind all the complicated stuff like transistors and GPS and such.
Replies from: Jayson_Virissimo, ChristianKl, ChristianKl↑ comment by Jayson_Virissimo · 2013-10-24T02:00:26.648Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
Pretty medieval, I think.
To be fair to the medievals, they did end up inventing the clock, windmills, spectacles, wheelbarrows, the longbow, astrolabes, chainmail, etc... without the help of scientists.
I don't see why engineers would discover and develop electricity, to start with, never mind all the complicated stuff like transistors and GPS and such.
Perhaps not, but the steam engine was invented by technologists without much input from academics and the first airplane was built at a time when many highly credentialed physicists were saying it was basically impossible. The "engineers just apply theories they get from scientists to concrete problems"-paradigm doesn't really fit the historical record. As often as not, the influence goes in the opposite direction.
Replies from: Jayson_Virissimo↑ comment by Jayson_Virissimo · 2013-10-24T02:44:36.652Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
Corrections: (1) The astrolabe was around since Hellenistic times, although the spherical astrolabe actually does date from the Middle Ages. (2) It seems likely that the spherical astrolabe was invented with input from "scientists" (natural philosophers).
↑ comment by ChristianKl · 2013-10-24T23:44:18.410Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
I don't see why engineers would discover and develop electricity, to start with, never mind all the complicated stuff like transistors and GPS and such.
Oskar Heil was an electrical engineers and at the same time one of the first people to get a patent for a transistor design.
↑ comment by ChristianKl · 2013-10-23T23:01:07.716Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
Because you're typing these words looking at a screen and the words are magically transported all around the world to appear at screens of other people...
How do you know that science is responsible?
comment by Desrtopa · 2013-10-23T15:35:11.488Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
In the short term, science may not be all that reliable, but keep in mind that we know that this particular data is unreliable because replication attempts have rendered it highly suspect.
In some areas, science is self correcting over a much longer time horizon than others. For fundamental physics, a comparably easy to correct misapprehension would be unlikely to persist for so long (in a modern day context.) In medicine, it might last much longer (because there are so many hypotheses to evaluate relative to the available research funds and manpower, and there's often inadequate communication between scientists who research stuff and doctors who implement it.) Psychology isn't the absolute bottom of the heap in terms of speed of correction, but it's well down there.
comment by passive_fist · 2013-10-23T23:42:43.885Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
In a nutshell, it's simply this: Stuff that people actually care about are more likely to be under intense scrutiny and thus more likely correct.
If you sift through the chemical literature, I guarantee you will find scores of papers that talk about compounds and synthesis methods that are false or bogus. It's not really important; no one ever cares about them enough to actually investigate the problems. Remember that passing peer review doesn't mean, "There is no doubt about this work." It just means, "Not obviously wrong; here it is exposed to the world and everyone is invited to look at it more deeply."
It's much the same in even more rigorous fields like math and physics. I have personally found serious errors in at least two math papers that have been published in respectable journals. Neither of them had any citations, otherwise people would have found the errors sooner. I was the first citation.
On the other hand, papers that have thousands of citations are more likely to be correct, simply because more people have looked at them.
comment by ephion · 2013-10-23T15:32:54.878Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
Pure science seems to be primarily a hypothesis generating mechanism. "Hey look at this thing we found! It might be cool and it might also be nothing." Engineering (or applied science) is a product generating mechanism. "So we tried a bunch of stuff, and now we've got this Somewhat Reliably Working Box. It does This Action when you push a button!"
Replies from: Lumifer, ChristianKl↑ comment by ChristianKl · 2013-10-23T23:16:21.523Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
Pure science seems to be primarily a hypothesis generating mechanism.
It's pretty bad at that task. Creating a hypothesis doesn't require a rigorous scientific experiment.
comment by Locaha · 2013-10-23T20:45:30.729Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
Science is a self-correcting mechanism. If I try to base my research on a paper that I can't replicate, I'd drop it and base the research on something else. So the reproducibility problem is not lethal.
Replies from: ChristianKl, Lumifer↑ comment by ChristianKl · 2013-10-24T00:18:54.314Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
One usually doesn't try to replicate all the research on which one bases ones paper. Even papers that fail at replication get cited widely.
comment by Eugene · 2013-10-24T08:23:02.878Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
I'm not involved in any science fields so for all I know this is a thing that exists, but if it is, it isn't discussed much: perhaps some scientific fields (or even all of them?) need an incentive for refuting other peoples' experiments. As far as I understand it, many experiments only ever get reproduced by a 3rd party when somebody needs it in order to build on their own hypothesis. So in other words, "so-and-so validated hypothesis X1 via this experiment. I have made hypothesis X2 which is predicated on X1's validity, so I'll reproduce the experiment before moving forward".
What if there was a journal dedicated to publishing research papers whose goal is purely to invalidate prior experiments? Or even more extreme, a Nobel prize in Invalidation? Could some fields be made more reliable if more people were put to the task of reproducing experiments?
comment by Ishaan · 2013-10-24T07:17:43.127Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
My tentative theory is that you don't need to be reliable to create progress. Exploratory behavior isn't supposed to e reliable - it's just supposed to succeed every once in a while. After the initial success, the reliable folks can take over and keep going until they hit a wall again.
There's an easy way to test this. Just pick a recent technology for which 1) you don't know the history 2) you don't really know how it works. Look around your room (or your head) and find any random example of recent, remarkable technology...and then check wikipedia. You've got to go roughly recent enough that not everyone was using them before 1990.
Prediction (before doing it): The pattern will emerge that the first crucial step was done by a scientist, and then later it was expanded into something useful by others.
I picked Pacemaker, LCD screen and contact lense. Reading these pages, I judge that the first, key breakthrough which made contacts and LCD possible were done by scientists. (In the case of the contact lens, the tech was there but impracticle until science made a breakthrough.). Pacemakers seems to be largely non-scientists.
Just keep picking various technologies at random and see the history to get a sense for the extent and nature of science's contribution to technology. Don't look at the reliability of new science.
Edit: Come to think of it, cardiac pacemaker might actually not be recent enough...
Replies from: ChristianKl↑ comment by ChristianKl · 2013-10-24T12:33:34.824Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
There's an easy way to test this. Just pick a recent technology for which 1) you don't know the history 2) you don't really know how it works. Look around your room (or your head) and find any random example of recent, remarkable technology...and then check wikipedia. You've got to go roughly recent enough that not everyone was using them before 1990.
Given that sentence, the first technology that springs to mind is wikipedia itself. The crucial step here seems to be the discovery that people actually start editing much more when you give them a wiki interface then when you ask them to send you their contributions via email.
It was an accidental discovery.
Prediction (before doing it): The pattern will emerge that the first crucial step was done by a scientist, and then later it was expanded into something useful by others.
From the outside it's not easy to know what happened to be the crucial step. There are certain things that are obvious when you tinker with a technology. A scientist has probably dealt with the effect years earlier but the engineer might not know about the work of the scientist.
It's quite often that people file patents for stuff that people can later discover independently. As far as the Wikipedia article for Pacemakers goes it's not clear that Mark C Lidwell knew about J A McWilliam or that McWilliams work was crucial for him.
The Wikipedia expertiment only shows you that scientists like to credit themselves and their collegues with advancing technology and that public culture supports them in that quest.
Science is optimized for taking credit for other peoples work. Scientists get payed for being credited. Engineers on the other hand get payed to produce useful technology.
Replies from: Ishaan↑ comment by Ishaan · 2013-10-24T19:35:22.879Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
Wikipedia breaks the "it's not immediately obvious how it works even when it's right in front of you" rule. A window does not pass this criteria, but glass passes. Once you have glass, you can expect people to innovate cool ways to use it...but you can't expect people to come up with glass making without any evidence to work from.
The reason for that rule is that scientists only come into the picture where the limiting factor is a lack of knowledge about the world. If it's a clever implementation of existing knowledge (like wikipedia), it's not science
Glass is fairly simple - at some level of complexity, you need a systematic knowledge of what you are working with to do useful stuff. Informal knowledge won't cut it. It's easy to point to some bad statistics, but stats are baby science taking first steps into a new field - in its mature "we're finally starting to get this" stages, science creates mechanisms which are highly predictive of reality. Finding correlations is just a method of knowing what direction to go.
Science is optimized for taking credit for the discovery. Engineering is optimized for taking credit for the invention. It's not like engineers are disinterested in who gets credit for the work...and the discovery and invention both get produced nonetheless. Although I suppose you are correct that popular culture (and thus the wiki page) might favor the scientist.
Replies from: ChristianKl↑ comment by ChristianKl · 2013-10-24T20:34:04.184Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
Glass is fairly simple - at some level of complexity, you need a systematic knowledge of what you are working with to do useful stuff. Informal knowledge won't cut it.
Glass is interesting. Obsidian is the first glass that got used and people got it because it occurs naturally and is highly useful. Wikipedia suggest that the first custom glass was made as an accidental byproduct of metalworking or in the production of faience. It doesn't suggest that it was created by someone doing science.
In Egypt people started to deal with glass the same way they dealt as gold and silver. This meaned that you got craftsman who made pretty objects with it. Those craftsman then gathered a lot of informal knowledge about it and after some time passes they make windows with it as they get better at dealing with it.
Engineering is optimized for taking credit for the invention.
It isn't. Most engineers are payed to create produced that get sold. If an engineer invents something and 20 years later another company brings the product to market the engineer gets nothing.
If a scientist discovers something and 20 years latter another person does something with it, the scientist get all the credit.
In general engineers aren't hired based on what they have invented in the past. Scientists on the other hand do get hired based on published papers with as supposed to reflect how much the scientist can be credit for increasing human knowledge.
There no equivalent to publish or perish for engineers.
comment by ShardPhoenix · 2013-10-24T00:09:15.988Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
You appear to be getting at the concept of Tacit Knowledge.
Replies from: NancyLebovitzCollins showed [10] that a particular laser (The ppTEA laser) was designed in America and the idea, with specific assistance from the designers, was gradually propagated to various other universities world-wide. However, in the early days, even when specific instructions were sent, other labs failed to replicate the laser, it only being made to work in each case following a visit to or from the originating lab or very close contact and dialogue. It became clear that while the originators could clearly make the laser work, they did not know exactly what it was that they were doing to make it work, and so could not articulate or specify it by means of monologue articles and specifications. But a cooperative process of dialogue enabled the tacit knowledge to be transferred.
↑ comment by NancyLebovitz · 2013-10-25T13:58:36.814Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
I expect that part of what I'm talking about is tacit knowledge, but I bet that some of it is explicit but localized knowledge. The knowledge (whether tacit or explicit) remains local partly because no one is interested in hearing about it-- too arcane for the lay public, too low status for scientists, partly because a lot of it is trade secrets and/or requires a security clearance, and partly because it's highly specific, which means it gets forgotten if a project or technology is abandoned.
comment by Lumifer · 2013-10-23T16:36:05.033Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
some fairly effective methods of filtering information that comes from science.
Sure, there is a fairly effective method called empirical testing. You build/make/create it and you see if it works in the real world.
Results from science tend to come as well-defined inside a narrow context and subject to specific assumptions and constraints. Reality is much messier so the scientific results, even if "true", might (and often do) turn out to have no practical value.