Fake Explanations

post by Eliezer Yudkowsky (Eliezer_Yudkowsky) · 2007-08-20T21:13:35.000Z · LW · GW · Legacy · 96 comments

Once upon a time, there was an instructor who taught physics students. One day the instructor called them into the classroom and showed them a wide, square plate of metal, next to a hot radiator. The students each put their hand on the plate and found the side next to the radiator cool, and the distant side warm. And the instructor said, Why do you think this happens? Some students guessed convection of air currents, and others guessed strange metals in the plate. They devised many creative explanations, none stooping so low as to say “I don’t know” or “This seems impossible.”

And the answer was that before the students entered the room, the instructor turned the plate around.1

Consider the student who frantically stammers, “Eh, maybe because of the heat conduction and so?” I ask: Is this answer a proper belief? The words are easily enough professed—said in a loud, emphatic voice. But do the words actually control anticipation?

Ponder that innocent little phrase, “because of,” which comes before “heat conduction.” Ponder some of the other things we could put after it. We could say, for example, “Because of phlogiston,” or “Because of magic.”

“Magic!” you cry. “That’s not a scientific explanation!” Indeed, the phrases “because of heat conduction” and “because of magic” are readily recognized as belonging to different literary genres. “Heat conduction” is something that Spock might say on Star Trek, whereas “magic” would be said by Giles in Buffy the Vampire Slayer.

However, as Bayesians, we take no notice of literary genres. For us, the substance of a model is the control it exerts on anticipation. If you say “heat conduction,” what experience does that lead you to anticipate? Under normal circumstances, it leads you to anticipate that, if you put your hand on the side of the plate near the radiator, that side will feel warmer than the opposite side. If “because of heat conduction” can also explain the radiator-adjacent side feeling cooler, then it can explain pretty much anything.

And as we all know by this point (I do hope), if you are equally good at explaining any outcome, you have zero knowledge. “Because of heat conduction,” used in such fashion, is a disguised hypothesis of maximum entropy. It is anticipation-isomorphic to saying “magic.” It feels like an explanation, but it’s not.

Suppose that instead of guessing, we measured the heat of the metal plate at various points and various times. Seeing a metal plate next to the radiator, we would ordinarily expect the point temperatures to satisfy an equilibrium of the diffusion equation with respect to the boundary conditions imposed by the environment. You might not know the exact temperature of the first point measured, but after measuring the first points—I’m not physicist enough to know how many would be required—you could take an excellent guess at the rest.

A true master of the art of using numbers to constrain the anticipation of material phenomena—a “physicist”—would take some measurements and say, “This plate was in equilibrium with the environment two and a half minutes ago, turned around, and is now approaching equilibrium again.”

The deeper error of the students is not simply that they failed to constrain anticipation. Their deeper error is that they thought they were doing physics. They said the phrase “because of,” followed by the sort of words Spock might say on Star Trek, and thought they thereby entered the magisterium of science.

Not so. They simply moved their magic from one literary genre to another.


1 Joachim Verhagen, Science Jokes, 2001, http://web.archive.org/web/20060424082937/http://www.nvon.nl/scheik/best/diversen/scijokes/scijokes.txt

96 comments

Comments sorted by oldest first, as this post is from before comment nesting was available (around 2009-02-27).

comment by Anonymous_Coward · 2007-08-21T00:23:18.000Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Well, one difference between "heat conduction" and "phlogiston" is that the former carries some additional information with it - heat conduction is a well-understood mechanism by which energy is transferred from place to place. Maybe it does apply in that situation and maybe it doesn't - in the example given, it doesn't, there's no heat-conduction mechanism to transfer heat from one side to the other - but the fact that there's actually a mechanism behind the words separates it, qualitatively, from an explanation like "phlogiston." It has equations behind it which can then be written down and tested for agreement with reality.

Really, I can quite understand the students... if you say "I don't know" you have a zero percent chance of getting the explanation right. If you say "that seems impossible," then you're guaranteed to get it 100% wrong - since it DID happen, and thus it must be possible. The best course of action in the situation is to think of all the hypotheses you can, and then guess at one of them - whichever one has the highest chance of being right, given what they know about physics.

Now, I certainly hope that the students wouldn't think that by throwing around guesses they're "doing physics" - yes, doing physics would involve taking actual measurements, and I would hope that after taking some measurements of the block over time they would see "oh, this isn't actually at equilibrium like we had all assumed." (Alternatively, if a student took the words and wrote down an actual model of how the air currents or the different metals or the heat conduction could lead to the observations, that would also be "doing physics", though the only end result of it would be to yield a mathematical model which would quickly be easy to proven false by measurements or stability analysis.) But neither of those avenues is open to them when they walk into a classroom and the teacher asks them to "explain this phenomenon."

I think the students would quite happily agree that they haven't given an explanation which is good by any sane measure - it's quite likely that many of them would also agree that they don't actually believe their explanations. But I wouldn't agree that they're being irrational in stating them.

Replies from: MrPineapple, orthonormal
comment by MrPineapple · 2011-01-24T01:07:34.878Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

"Eh, maybe because of the heat conduction and so?"

should give you at least 1/3 of a point, after all, one side of the plate is hot :)

Replies from: Desrtopa, johnlawrenceaspden
comment by Desrtopa · 2011-01-24T01:25:28.273Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

This is exactly the sort of mistake that Guessing the Teacher's Password was written to disabuse. The answer demonstrates no understanding of the phenomenon.

comment by johnlawrenceaspden · 2012-08-27T14:12:09.723Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Upvoted. Heat conduction is involved in the true answer. The student has demonstrated that he at least knows where to start thinking about the question. I'd have more respect for someone who said 'heat conduction', or for that matter 'caloric flow',and tried to work out how that might work than someone who said 'fairies'. Given that this is a question from a physics teacher, you're not wrong to try to find physics explanations before you go to 'trickery', which is exactly the sort of 'explain everything' hypothesis that we're nervous of.

comment by orthonormal · 2011-01-24T01:29:13.633Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Everyone agrees that the physics students are just doing what they've been incentivized to do in class after class. It's just worth pointing out that the behavior they've been trained to do is not at all like doing science, and that nobody seems to know or worry about this.

comment by Eliezer Yudkowsky (Eliezer_Yudkowsky) · 2007-08-21T01:01:18.000Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

AC, what you're describing here is a severe case of déformation educationnelle.

Really, I can quite understand the students... if you say "I don't know" you have a zero percent chance of getting the explanation right.

If you say "I don't know" you have a zero percent chance of getting a gold star in the idiot damned school system. But it is still the rational thing to say when, in fact, you don't know. You can easily do worse than maximum entropy if you guess at random.

Furthermore, "getting it right" by guessing the verbal phrase the teacher has in mind, even if the school system gives you a gold star for it, does not necessarily mean that you possess any anticipation-controllers. All you got right was a string of words, like guessing the passphrase to the teacher's login.

"Heat conduction" is a verbal phrase which may, for someone who knows the equations, invoke genuinely explanatory equations from memory. And for someone who knows the equations, it should be obvious that the equations do not predict the further side being warmer.

If you don't know the equations, then "heat conduction" is a verbal phrase invoking magic from the Star Trek genre. Even if the teacher says "You're right!", because there were only a limited number of phrases you learned about this semester and one of them had to be "right", you still don't know anything except an arbitrary passphrase.

If you memorized the equations but you didn't apply them, then "heat conduction" still invokes magic - it's not enough to know what symbols to scribble on the final exam, you've got to do the math or it doesn't count.

Replies from: royf
comment by royf · 2012-06-02T07:13:32.531Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

You are overstating the case by a large margin.

[Saying "I don't know"] is still the rational thing to say when, in fact, you don't know.

Saying "I don't know" may be, to a large degree, the true state of your belief when you use probability theory. But in this case it's not the rational thing to say when you use decision theory. "I don't know" is true, but it is a non-answer to the question, and doesn't get you points. It's a different matter whether this point system is effective or moral, but as long as it's there, that's what you play by.

You can easily do worse than maximum entropy if you guess at random.

If you base your guess correctly on an incomplete model of reality, which you've constructed correctly from past observations, you can never do worse, on average, than maximum entropy. More evidence can never lead to less information (as per the Data Processing Inequality).

Furthermore, "getting it right" [...] does not necessarily mean that you possess any anticipation-controllers.

On the contrary, it mean exactly that. Being rewarded for predictive powers improves your model of the world, whereas "I don't know" is an excuse for not knowing.

In fact, the mechanism employed by the teacher, for all its flaws, achieves 3 important goals:

  • It motivates students to pay attention, raises their level of alertness, activates their brains.
  • It rewards students to engage their past observations to generate the most accurate belief they can about the right answer. In the process, they build a better model of the world, and they make their unknown unknowns a little more known.
  • By forcing students to generate a belief and commit to it before the correct answer is revealed, their hindsight bias is reduced.
Replies from: Chrysophylax
comment by Chrysophylax · 2013-01-29T22:58:09.824Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

But in this case it's not the rational thing to say when you use decision theory.

I disagree. The proper response to not knowing the answer is to admit to not knowing and then give your best guess, not to try to hide your ignorance, because if you succeed then the teacher doesn't know you need help. A student who is more concerned with not displaying ignorance than with not being ignorant is not trying to learn, which is not rational. That which can be destroyed by the truth should be, and it probably won't be if you try to avoid finding out what the truth is.

You can easily do worse than maximum entropy if you guess at random.

If you base your guess correctly on an incomplete model of reality, which you've constructed correctly from past observations, you can never do worse, on average, than maximum entropy. More evidence can never lead to less information (as per the Data Processing Inequality).

The key phrase here is "on average". If you guess at random from all possible explanations of a given phenomenon, you will, on average, die before guessing the correct answer. There is a reason the monkeys with typewriters are given infinite time to reproduce Hamlet.

Moreover, as the set of answers considered increases in size, the expected utility from giving any one answer tends towards the expected utility of a wrong answer. As long as giving the wrong answer gives less utility than admitting ignorance, admitting ignorance is almost always the utility maximising option if you don't know.

Furthermore, "getting it right" [...] does not necessarily mean that you possess any anticipation-controllers.

On the contrary, it mean exactly that. Being rewarded for predictive powers improves your model of the world, whereas "I don't know" is an excuse for not knowing.

If I write down a number and then take a number from a table of random numbers, and the numbers are the same, does this mean that I'm psychic? Because if getting the correct answer means that I have useful anticipation controllers then I must be.

"I don't know" is not an excuse for not knowing. That makes no sense whatsoever. "I don't know" is a statement about whether I know something or not, not a statement about whether I ought to know. If you can't admit fallibility then you will never learn anything.

The points you make about the benefits of testing students' knowledge are true. Unfortunately, they miss the point - while it is important not to penalise guessing incorrectly, so as not to dissuade admitting ignorance, it is much better to actively reward admitting that you have tried and failed. If a confused student does not always seek an explanation, the reward for seeking explanations isn't large enough yet. If students are content to remain ignorant, something is seriously wrong with your system for making students less ignorant.

Replies from: V_V, whowhowho
comment by V_V · 2013-01-30T00:45:38.965Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

If students could always get away with an "I don't know" they wouldn't have much incentive to learn anything.

More importantly, the school system main purpose is not to teach you just a collection of facts. It has to teach you how to behave in the world, where you often have to make choices based on incomplete information.

Replies from: wedrifid, Chrysophylax
comment by wedrifid · 2013-01-30T00:52:04.892Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

If students could always get away with an "I don't know" they wouldn't have much incentive to learn anything.

0 marks for "I don't know". 1 mark for a correct answer. -1 mark for an incorrect answer.

Not only is it a simple incentive system I've done exams that implemented similar systems. (Westpac math competition for example.)

Replies from: V_V, Decius
comment by V_V · 2013-01-30T00:58:22.242Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

That is a sensible scoring system which is in fact widely used.

Replies from: Chrysophylax
comment by Chrysophylax · 2013-01-30T18:26:38.419Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

This is, in fact, close to being the worst system ever devised. The fact that something is widely used does not mean that it is any good. Examining the results of this kind of system shows that, when applied to unfamilliar material, they consistently give the best marks to the worst students. If the best students can't do every problem with extreme ease, they tend to venture answers where poor students do not. This results in the best students dropping towards the median score and the highest scores going to poor students who were lucky. Applying the system to familliar material should produce a similar, though less pronounced, effect. Adding penalties lowers the dispersion about the mean, which always makes an exam less useful.

Exam systems that have no penalty for wrong answers are better than ones that do, but are still imperfect. The only reliable way to guage students ability is to have far more questions (preferably taken as several papers), to reduce the effect of mistakes relative to ignorance and to increase the number of areas examined. This is generally cost-prohibitive. It also tests students' ability to answer exam questions, rather than testing their understanding. There is, fortunately, a way to test understanding - a student understands material when they can rediscover the ideas that draw on it.

Replies from: Vaniver
comment by Vaniver · 2013-01-30T18:46:55.830Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

This is, in fact, close to being the worst system ever devised.

Not really- it teaches calibration as well as correctness. Are you more than 50% sure? No? Then don't guess.

In fact, it shares several properties with the best system ever devised (for multiple choice questions, at least): the test-taker assigns a probability to each of the answers (and the total probability doled out must sum to one), and is graded based on the logarithm of the probability they assigned to the correct answer. (Typically, there's an offset so that assigning equal probability to all possibilities gives a score of 0, so that it is possible to get positive points.)

Examining the results of this kind of system shows that, when applied to unfamilliar material, they consistently give the best marks to the worst students.

Do you have linkable results? My experience with the probability log-scoring is that, even on the first test, the median score is somewhat better than 0, there are several negative scorers, but the test-takers who received the best marks (who are both high-accuracy and high-calibration) are noticeably different from the pack, and are hardly the worst students.

The worst marks often go to students whose accuracy is high, but whose calibration is low, but that goes away once they learn calibration, which seems like a feature, not a bug.

If the best students can't do every problem with extreme ease, they tend to venture answers where poor students do not. This results in the best students dropping towards the median score and the highest scores going to poor students who were lucky.

How can poor students get lucky if they don't venture answers to questions where they are not sure?

The only reliable way to guage students ability is to have far more questions (preferably taken as several papers), to reduce the effect of mistakes relative to ignorance and to increase the number of areas examined.

The trouble with this approach is that you then are also grading speed and resistance to mental fatigue. In some cases, that is desirable; in others, not.

comment by Decius · 2013-01-30T01:15:53.983Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Allow both an answer and a certainty.

-x points for an incorrect answer with certainty x
+2x points for the correct answer with certainty x

Alternately, +10^x points for a correct answer with certainty x, and +Log(1-x) points for the incorrect answer. This encourages an attempt to answer every question, even if the certainty is rated as 0.

Replies from: Jiro
comment by Jiro · 2014-08-18T19:11:12.204Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Yes, I know, old post.

If you give the student -X points for an incorrect answer with certainty X, and +2X points for a correct answer with certainty X, the expected value of giving an answer and lying about its certainty as Y is (1-X)(-Y) + (X)(2Y) = 3XY - Y. If X is less than 1/3, the student should lie and claim that his certainty is 0, while if X is greater than 1/3, he should lie and claim that his certainty is 1.

I'm not going to try to find the maximum for the second version, but it should be obvious that the student is still better off lying about his true certainty. Of course, you could just avoid telling the student how you're going to grade, but the score will then just depend on how well the student guesses your grading criteria.

Replies from: Decius
comment by Decius · 2014-09-17T04:03:39.710Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Neither of my described systems are ideal. Squared error works for binary questions, but it would reward "Pi is exactly 3, with 0 confidence".

Rather than allow continuous estimates of accuracy, I think that the ideal system would ask the student to provide a range of confidence, (five choices from "guessing" to "Certain", with equivalent probabilities), and an appropriate scoring rule; a guess would be penalized 0 for being wrong but gain little for being right, and going from "almost certain" to "certain" would add a small value to a correct answer but a large penalty to a wrong answer.

Having established the +points for correct and -points for wrong for each confidence description, do the math to determine what the actual ranges of confidence are, sanity check them against the descriptions, and then tell the student the confidence intervals. (Alternately, pick the intervals and terms and do the math to figure out the + for correct answer and -for incorrect answer for those intervals.)

Replies from: Jiro
comment by Jiro · 2014-09-17T14:47:01.629Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

and going from "almost certain" to "certain" would add a small value to a correct answer but a large penalty to a wrong answer.

It's hard to come up with a system where the student doesn't benefit from lying about his certainty. What you describe would fix the case from 4 (almost certain) to 5 (certain), but you need to get all the cases to work and it's plausible that fixing the 4 to 5 case (and, in general, increasing the incentive to pick 4) breaks the 3 to 4 case.

After all, you can't have all the transitions between certainty values add a small value to a correct answer. You must have a transition where a large value is added for a correct answer and your system may break down around such transitions.

Replies from: Decius
comment by Decius · 2014-09-18T01:13:42.752Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

The largest value would be added for the first confidence interval, which would also add the smallest cost to being wrong with that confidence.

Replies from: Jiro
comment by Jiro · 2014-09-18T01:37:24.162Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

That would mean a large value would be added when going from "guess" to "almost guess", which would mean that it would be beneficial for a student to lie and claim to almost guess when he's really completely guessing.

Replies from: Decius
comment by Decius · 2014-09-18T02:31:29.373Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Suppose the student thinks that there is a 10% chance that he is right, and the reward structure is +5/-1 for confidence interval 1.

In fact, make the reward structure:(right/wrong) 1/0, 6/-1, 10/-3, 13/-6, 15/-10, 16/-15

That puts the breakpoints at roughly even intervals, keeps the math easy, and with a little bit of clarifying exactly where the breakpoints are, doesn't reward someone who accurately determines their accuracy and then lies about it.

Replies from: Jiro
comment by Jiro · 2014-09-18T14:42:32.080Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

I sat down late last night trying to prove that this couldn't work and instead proved that it could. If I did this correctly, in order for it to work, with the confidences increasing from 0 to 1,

left side confidence <= (difference in Y)/(difference in X + difference in Y)

right side confidence >= (difference in Y)/(difference in X + difference in Y).

Differences in X are 5, 4, 3, 2, 1 and differences in Y are 1, 2, 3, 4, 5 leading to values of 1/6 through 5/6; as 0 < 1/6 < 1/5 < 2/6 < 2/5 < 3/6 < 3/5 < 4/6 < 4/5 < 5/6 < 1 this is immune to lying within a single interval (and also turns out to be so for multiple intervals).

Replies from: Decius
comment by Decius · 2014-09-19T06:27:02.402Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

So, what are the downsides of making this a grading standard? The biggest one I see is that it would be unfair except in classes that have as prerequisites an outstanding score in a class that covers credence calibration.

comment by Chrysophylax · 2013-01-30T18:51:34.223Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Students who do not care about education do get away with not knowing anything. Detention is not much of a punishment when you don't show up.

It is difficult to prevent a student who cares deeply about eduction from admitting ignorance, since admitting ignorance is necessary in asking for explanations. The difficult task is persuading students who care about doing well to seek knowledge, rather than good marks. These students are not motivated enough to learn of their own accord - they never volunteer answers or ask questions openly, because they care more about not being thought ignorant (or, of course, keen) than about not being ignorant.

The point is not to allow students to "get away with" admitting ignorance. There is a vast difference between not knowing the answer and not wanting to know. Personally, I have never found it hard to tell the difference between students who don't want to know and students who don't want to be judged by their peers.

It has to teach you how to behave in the world, where you often have to make choices based on incomplete information.

It is very rarely a bad idea to publicly admit that you might be wrong, especially when you are guessing. A school that does not teach the importance of separating your beliefs and your ego has failed miserably. Whatever else it has taught, it has not taught its students how to learn.

comment by whowhowho · 2013-01-30T01:24:43.921Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

If you guess at random from all possible explanations of a given phenomenon, you will, on average, die before guessing the correct answer

How true

comment by Alexis_Perrier · 2007-08-21T07:26:45.000Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Dear Sir, Although your article follows the essence of logic, may I point out that Mr Spock, a personal mentor, has never had anything to do with misleading scientific explanations. Mr Spock just happens to live in the future where such things as "plasma conduits" and "warp engines" are common scientific tools. Yours truly Alexis Perrier

comment by ed_johnson · 2007-08-21T08:07:39.000Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

I agree with AC...you're being too hard on the students. I doubt very much they were stating anything with confidence. It's quite possible that some of them didn't really care about understanding physics and were just trying to get the right answer to please the teacher, but others were probably just thinking out loud. Thinking "maybe it's heat conduction" might just be the first step to thinking "no, it can't be heat conduction," or even to realizing "I don't really understand heat conduction," and there is nothing wrong with this train of thought. They were probably "biased" towards the idea that there was some physical principle causing the effect, but that was entirely rational because the professor set them up to believe that.

Great story, though.

Replies from: DilGreen
comment by DilGreen · 2010-10-01T14:32:50.117Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

I think that EY's problem with this point of view is a typical one that I find here at LW: a consideration of the rational thinker as loner in heroic mode, who is expected to ignore all contexts (social, environmental, whatever) that are not explicitly stated as part of the problem presentation. On the other hand, these students were in a physics class, and the question is obviously not part of normal conversation.

Replies from: matteyas
comment by matteyas · 2017-08-03T20:59:45.828Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Are you saying that in an environment for learning about- and discussing rationality, we should strive for a less-than-ideal rationality (that is, some form of irrationality) just because of practical contexts that people often run into and choose the easy way out of?

Would you become equally suspicious of the math teacher's point of view if some person from a math problem buys 125 boxes with 6 watermelons each, since he won't be able to handle that amount in most practical contexts?

comment by Senthil · 2007-08-21T10:09:02.000Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Ed, the student's response may be due to something he needs to unlearn as discussed in the following earlier post:

http://lesswrong.com/lw/i2/two_more_things_to_unlearn_from_school/

If it's not the case, that is, if he doesn't need to unlearn anything he may still be incorrect in his understanding. In that case, this post tells one of the reasons why he may be incorrect and be aware of it.

I think this is worse compared to the behaviour addressed in the earlier post.

comment by Stuart_Armstrong · 2007-08-21T10:19:12.000Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

but that was entirely rational because the professor set them up to believe that.

They were rational, but not unbiased. They wanted to maximise their chances of pleasing the prof., not maximise their chances of understanding the world.

I think this teaching approach was great, and I might use something similar myself (there are mathematical equivalents of the above situation). Learning science means that you have to learn a boatload of facts, and learn the scientific method. Since the boatload of facts has to be accepted without question (for the whole of your early career), this undermines the teaching of the method (when it is taught at all). A few sessions like this (properly exploited by the instructor) would do a world of good.

Replies from: johnlawrenceaspden
comment by johnlawrenceaspden · 2012-08-27T14:22:44.925Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Hmm, the boatload of facts (and the theories behind them) explain a larger boatload of facts that you already know. I found physics and maths very clear and easy and exciting because of this.

Despite my first love in infancy being chemistry, I abandoned it when they wanted me to memorise the colours of the transition metal ions. If they'd told me instead how those colours came about from the quantum theory, and shown me all the pretty colours by actually burning the damned things, instead of turning them into despicable rote work, I might have grown up to be a chemist.

comment by Hopefully_Anonymous · 2007-08-21T11:09:47.000Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Great post, Eliezer, and I agree with Stuart. There should be no valor in stating an uncertain guess as a certain statement -one should at least express one's level of uncertainty.

comment by Hopefully_Anonymous · 2007-08-21T11:11:59.000Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Incidentally, this is an area where legal instruction is superior to scientific instruction at the graduate/pre-thesis level.

Replies from: gwern, johnlawrenceaspden
comment by gwern · 2010-07-02T05:50:19.700Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

How so?

Replies from: AtreidesOne
comment by AtreidesOne · 2016-11-29T06:09:20.494Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Perhaps due to the presumption of innocence? They are constantly aware that you have to have proof beyond reasonable doubt to convict someone, whereas in other fields we are more likely to assume an answer exists?

comment by johnlawrenceaspden · 2012-08-27T14:26:06.692Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

A bit late, but I'd like an answer to this too. I enjoyed talking to law students at college. They clearly have the same sort of minds as mathematicians, except they can also talk like proper humans do, and their problems are interesting. If they have a better way of teaching that sort of thing, maybe someone should try using it for science.

Replies from: Phoenix_Wright
comment by Phoenix_Wright · 2012-12-23T01:16:16.970Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

I think perhaps the reason one would say that legal education is better is that it is understood from the first day that many of the problems that will be posed actually have no answer ("What is justice?" "How can we balance the interests in this scenario?" "What would the reasonable [sic] person do given this dilemma?") and that what is important is the quality of the reasoning you use to come to your answer, not the outcome.

When a well-argued, incorrect answer is scored more highly than a correct answer with no justification, the get-the-gold-star incentive is removed and it improves quality of thought on the matter.

Maybe this person meant something entirely different; I can't claim to speak on their behalf.

Replies from: Desrtopa
comment by Desrtopa · 2013-05-09T14:34:55.471Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

It would be no surprise if legal training turns out clever arguers, but there's a big difference between arguing persuasively and getting the right answer. Training in questions where there is no single right answer may improve students' rhetoric, but I think it's likely to leave them underprepared when they have to weigh in on questions where there is a single right answer, and no amount of argument will make any other answer acceptable.

comment by conchis · 2007-08-21T12:53:45.000Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

"They wanted to maximise their chances of pleasing the prof., not maximise their chances of understanding the world."

I don't know that I buy this. If the students make a guess that's wrong, one would expect that to kickstart a process of the professor helping them to understand why it's wrong. (Student: "Um... because of heat conduction?" Teacher: "OK, what does heat conduction suggest should happen in this situation?"...) This seems more likely to result in learning than just sitting there and saying "I don't know". If anything, I think it's often a bigger problem from a learning perspective, when people are too afraid of being wrong to put out tentative ideas.

"I don't know" is a rational response to this situation if you are sure enough of your understanding of all the potential principles involved that you know they can't explain the phenomenon (and you don't happen to guess that the professor is messing with you). But it's fairly clear the students aren't in that situation, so starting to generate hypotheses about what's going on seems perfectly sensible. Of course, they should be actual hypotheses, and Eliezer's perfectly right that "because of heat conduction", if offered as an actual explanation, isn't an hypothesis as much as a cop out. But if it's a starting point, rather than an endpoint, then that seems perfectly reasonable.

In short, the problem isn't that they're guessing. It's if their guesses aren't actually saying anything, but they think that they are. (And I think Eliezer's admonition to just say "I don't know" conflates these two problems.)

Replies from: DSimon
comment by DSimon · 2013-06-19T13:49:24.484Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

How about "I don't know, but maybe it has something to do with X?"

comment by Byrne · 2007-08-21T15:00:16.000Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Is there any way to set up a classroom (or an educational system) so that these students would get the right answer? Alternatively, is it even desirable?

If you teach students to think this way, you're saying "The world is governed by comprehensible scientific laws -- which is irrelevant, because people are constantly screwing with you." This experiment might be useful in a physics class for lawyers (who would probably catch on) or conspiracy theorists (who would, at least, have more entertaining hypotheses).

A compromise might be for the teacher to not just ask for an explanation, but ask for a testable explanation, and reward people for coming up with a theory they can falsify. The sad truth is that there's one more group that would figure out the answer right away: Creationists.

Replies from: Tyrrell_McAllister, MixedNuts
comment by Tyrrell_McAllister · 2011-06-22T05:11:56.604Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

If you teach students to think this way, you're saying "The world is governed by comprehensible scientific laws -- which is irrelevant, because people are constantly screwing with you."

You are teaching them that, if they understand the scientific laws, they can catch the people constantly screwing with them.

comment by MixedNuts · 2011-06-22T06:37:04.788Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

there's one more group that would figure out the answer right away: Creationists.

Let's test this. Now. Who has access to a group of creationists, a group of evolutionists, a room with a heater and a metal plate?

comment by Eliezer Yudkowsky (Eliezer_Yudkowsky) · 2007-08-21T16:08:12.000Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

I should note that I read about this scenario in the Canonical List of Science Jokes but I have no idea whether it was a note from someone's experience. If anyone tries this, I'd love to know the result - my guess is that in real life, at least one student in the class would guess it, which is why I'd suggest having students write down their best suggestions on paper; followed by the teacher asking "How could you falsify your theory?" and writing that down as well.

Explaining things by magic has been the default state of human existence for far longer than science ever existed. Anyone using fancy words must be assumed to be invoking magic by default.

The training of a rationalist must be strict! No human can be unfair enough; you have to match swords against Nature to develop the requisite skills.

comment by Bob3 · 2007-08-21T17:55:06.000Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

I would like to re-emphasize Eliezer's point that "I don't know" (not an uneducated guess) is the proper answer to any question where, in fact, a student (or person in general) does not know the right answer, with the addition of "but I will find out." On my exams a (fully) incorrect answer gets zero points while "I don't know but I will find out" gets one-half credit. You still fail if you don't know anything, but at least you are not in an idiot damned school system. Rewarding students for data dumps when they don't know an answer cannot be healthy. Or maybe I'm just biased because my approach makes exam grading significantly easier....

Replies from: gjayb
comment by gjayb · 2011-06-16T20:24:55.433Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Out of curiosity: do you mean that you give students credit for professing that they will find out? Or do you have them take the problems home sometime later, look things up, do the work, and then give them half the points back? Because I have seen the latter work very well, while I would see the former as once again asking students to put down what their teachers want to hear.

Replies from: johnlawrenceaspden
comment by johnlawrenceaspden · 2012-08-27T14:45:12.734Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

That's a wonderful idea. Timed exams but you can get (tiny amounts) of extra credit for handing in the answers later in the day having asked your friends.

Screws it up as an intelligence test (which may be the real point of formal exams), but would do wonders for learning if you did it on regular informal tests.

Replies from: viktor-riabtsev-1
comment by Viktor Riabtsev (viktor-riabtsev-1) · 2018-10-21T20:33:57.385Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

My favorite thing to do in physics/math classes, all the way up 2nd year in university (I went into engineering), was to ask others how they fared on tests, (in order to) then try to figure out why my answers were wrong.

I found genuine pleasure in understanding where I went wrong. Yet this seemed taboo in highschool, and (slightly less) frowned upon in university.

I feel like rewarding the student who messed up, however much or little, with some fraction of the total test score, like 10%; would be a great idea. You gain incentive to figure out what you missed; even if you care little about it. That's better then nothing.

comment by conchis · 2007-08-21T20:40:39.000Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Bob, Unless guessing is part of finding out. (This clearly isn't the case in an exam, but often is in a classroom situation.)

comment by Robin_Hanson2 · 2007-08-21T20:56:04.000Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Eliezer, I hope you are considering writing a book based on this excellent series of essays you have been writing.

comment by Eliezer Yudkowsky (Eliezer_Yudkowsky) · 2007-08-21T21:57:53.000Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Conchis, the problem is guessing passphrases instead of anticipation-controllers.

Robin, I am indeed considering it, it will depend on how much raw material I can generate.

comment by Shakespeare's_Fool · 2007-08-22T02:43:27.000Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

A student who said it was done by magic would, of course, have been correct. Because it was done by magic.

The teacher moved the plate when the audience wasn't looking. That is one of the ways magicians perform their tricks.

If they had used words such as "supernatural," "miracle," or "paranormal," then they would not have been discussing physics.

But good magicians are the best practical physicists.

comment by Senthil · 2007-08-22T05:48:54.000Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Eliezer, are you also considering giving free copies of the book to people who frequent this blog? :-)

comment by conchis · 2007-08-22T12:02:42.000Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Eliezer, Agreed. (That was my original point.)

comment by beowulflovesgrendel · 2007-08-27T03:32:11.000Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

The larger experiment seems to me to be the teacher's looking for someone with an answer to the ENTIRE 'experiment' which includes a 'false' set up. This isn't about 'physics,' it's about overall discernment, much the same way a truly observant participant will 'see through a magic trick,' no mean feat. So, for me, "I don't know" is the only honest and complete answer. It denotes an empty glass which (at least) can be filled and restricts that answwer to a particular observer and does not make 'unknowable" a universal state. Answers which are "data dumps" lull the mind into not questing further. You cannot 'wake up' until you know you have been 'asleep.' AND THEN, there is power imbalance. In a really optimal teaching environment, any 'student' (regardless of age, sex etc.) could forthrightly ask of any 'teacher,' "Is this a set up?" How rare is that?

comment by beowulflovesgrendel · 2007-08-27T03:34:06.000Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

regretting the above typos

comment by ron · 2007-08-27T07:14:58.000Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

I find it difficult to believe that none of the students would have guessed that the plate was turned around.

Replies from: Benevolence
comment by Benevolence · 2012-07-10T20:06:47.399Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Or is this just hindsight bias?

Edit: im a fool, new to posting on LW, just noticed the date. Point still stands though (not that i expect a reply)

Replies from: DaFranker
comment by DaFranker · 2012-07-10T20:19:14.256Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

You are not a fool... or so I want to believe anyway.

The Welcome to LessWrong page tells us that it's fine to just resume discussion even on old posts if you think there's something to add, and that sometimes new discussions started this way can be worth more than "not wasting your time" replying to old comments. Though of course, if you want a reply from the original author of the comment, you might want to first check if that user is still around, I reckon.

Replies from: Benevolence
comment by Benevolence · 2012-07-11T00:17:47.599Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Ahh, thank you for the link DaFranker.

comment by Wyrd_Fuquad · 2009-01-18T04:28:51.000Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

At one of the websites I frequent the first paragraph of this article was posted.

I guessed the teacher had set up the plate and turned it around.

As a student of the theatre I am somewhat versed in the arts of "Illusion". There was one show where we set off a smoke bomb and lowered the lights at the end of the first act. We then had intermission At the beginning of the second act we set of a smoke bomb and raised the lights. It's amazing how many people wrote about how we made the prop appear out of nowhere on the stage. They edited the intermission out of their remembrance of the story.

This story quickly sprang to my mind and I realized that if you can fool someone into forgetting an intermission you can fool someone that had no idea what was done before they came into the room in the first place.

comment by Todd_Non-Coward · 2009-05-17T00:50:05.000Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Eliezer, I have to disagree with some of what you wrote. The question was phrased as to give what you thought would be an answer. In such a setting "I don't know" would not be something to say -- silence would be the result. Additionally, if someone said "heat conductance?" they're not saying they DO know, but it's clearly a guess... and possibly without the question mark in their tone, they really are throwing it out as a guess with little or no confidence, while not believing with any confidence that it's the reason. Additionally, the setting is a 'trick', as the environment is set where the authority figure to teach is doing something in which there is an answer as to why that would happen without switching the plate before they entered the room. But, I would agree, that any kid with confidence saying something like "heat conductance", is a poor thing to do.

I don't think answering with "heat conductance" would be the same as magic if you didn't know the equations. I would say it would be like answering as "magic" if you didn't know what heat conductance was (the principal behind it). "Magic" is basically saying "no natural cause"... in a sense saying it's an unknown reason that you, Miss Physics teacher can't understand either. :)

comment by Dmytry · 2009-12-03T17:45:46.735Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Phew. This is just an 'ask a stupid question, get a stupid answer' situation.

Questions 'why' and 'what is' are metaphysical or semantic and have nothing whatsoever to do with science. The only reason why those are prevalent in education is that education sucks. Science is not a search for answers to "why X happens", even though it is popularized as such.

My school physics wasn't like this at all (eastern Europe here). I would have a problem to solve - how many watts of heating are required to maintain uniform temperature of 20 degrees Celsius in a 10x5x2.2m room with concrete walls of 20cm thickness and outside temperature of -20 degrees Celsius (you can look up thermal conductivity of concrete). A very reasonable question. In the end, you're good in physics not when you have some warm inner feeling of understanding, not when you can 'explain' why something happens, but when you can PREDICT what will happen and/or make things happen. Nature couldn't care less if you know or not "why" stone axe is good for killing a mammoth (or something), it doesn't care if you call it 'inertia' or something else.

Replies from: royf, johnlawrenceaspden
comment by royf · 2012-06-02T07:35:16.527Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

I agree that the word "why" is perhaps not the best choice, but it really stands for "what is the physical mechanism for".

Anyway, what you describe is engineering, not science. A scientist explains the mechanism by which a stone ax can kill a mammoth. An engineer uses this understanding to design a better ax. A craftsman builds the ax. A warrior takes the ax, kills the mammoth, and wins the girl.

comment by johnlawrenceaspden · 2012-08-27T14:56:03.136Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Questions 'why' and 'what is' are metaphysical or semantic and have nothing whatsoever to do with science.

And yet without them, science barely seems worth doing. I mean, someone finds out a new and seemingly useless fact about gravity and their name goes down in history. Someone makes a slight improvement to a mobile phone and they're not even famous in their own company.

comment by DilGreen · 2010-10-01T14:16:28.555Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Interestingly enough, my teacher, Chris. Alexander (author of A Pattern Language), recounts his entrance test for a physics degree at Cambridge. The applicants were asked to experimentally determine the magnetic field of the earth. He performed the experiment, and came up with an answer he knew to be wrong. Wrong by too large a margin to put down to experimental error. A smart chap, he had time to repeat the key part of the experiment, and recalculate - got the same answer. He used the last part of his time to write down his hypothesis for having achieved such a result. And, alone among the students, he was right. A massive electro-magnet was being used on the floor below as part of another experiment.

I believe the advice offered to me as an 18yr old physics student encountering similar circumstances was simply to show my workings and the incorrect result, and to add that I knew this was not the 'right' answer.

Replies from: Vladimir_Nesov
comment by Vladimir_Nesov · 2011-07-13T21:42:48.639Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

I had a similar episode in (Russian equivalent of) 10th grade, where a physics class lab experiment had critically flawed equipment, but we were supposed to write down all the steps according to a predetermined script described in the textbook. I instead described what was really happening in the experiment, why, and what was different from the intended scenario. The teacher marked other students according to how well they adhered to the script, even though it didn't square with the actual experimental data in any way, and they had to forge or "reinterpret" the data. (I did get an A, but possibly only because of my prodigy status.)

comment by Vladimir_Nesov · 2011-07-13T20:57:02.005Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

I have a bad memory for isolated facts (like names or past events), and comparatively poor ability to guess intended meaning of things people are talking about in real time. When I was younger, I would just randomly guess possible answers to patch over the gaps in my mental picture (with little chance for actual success) in situations where it was expected of me to know the answer. Generation of random explanations that have nothing to do with actually explaining the observations might sometimes be motivated by a similar psychological pressure to give some answer.

Now I say "I don't know" or "I don't understand", to the bewilderment of people who would expect better mental agility of me, but don't know me well enough.

comment by Douglas_Reay · 2012-02-19T18:17:54.626Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

I not so sure that when the student suggests "because of heat conduction", they are attempting to provide a full explanation.

I model their internal thinking more along the lines of "Well, I don't know for sure what's going on here, this is an obscure effect I've not come across before, but it seems plausible to me that it will be in some way connected with conduction, so I'll suggest that as a first step, and hope someone else can fill in the mathematical details for me."

It is closer to the situation when a company owner says to her managers "Production has dropped by 50% in the last quarter, what's the cause and what can we do about this?" and a manger replies "Well, I don't know for sure, but I think it might have something to do with the recent marketing campaign where we changed the product name to 'Yucko'."

Replies from: johnlawrenceaspden
comment by johnlawrenceaspden · 2012-08-27T14:58:37.086Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

hmm. surely that would affect sales first?

comment by johnlawrenceaspden · 2012-08-27T14:40:25.896Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

I heard this in the popular 'Oxbridge Interview Question' genre, a long time ago. It actually makes great sense there, as a 1960s don would have had a coal fire burning in winter, when the interviews are done, and be able to turn the plate round between interviews. And the interviewer would be expecting everyone to know all the relevant laws, and be looking for exactly the right level of bewildered confusion and hypothesis generation that you're hoping for.

The point is not to guess the right answer (that's essentially random inspiration and the ability to detect trickery is irrelevant for physics). The point is to provoke a detailed conversation about physics.

Some of these stories are clearly apocryphal, and some of them have the ring of truth. This one seems a bit dodgy since the previous interview candidate could easily spread the information about the trick (and although that's rather irrational, candidates do do this). Although if a candidate guessed straight away, the interviewer could just use another stock question.

comment by aspera · 2012-10-09T04:22:39.321Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Unless I misunderstand, this story is a parable. EY is communicating with a handwaving example that the effectiveness of a code doesn't depend on the alphabet used. In the code used to describe the plate phenomenon, “magic” and “heat conduction” are interchangeable symbols which formally carry zero information, since the coder doesn't use them to discriminate among cases.

I’m sincerely confused as to why comments center on the motivations of the students and the professor. Isn't that irrelevant? Or did EY mean for the discussion to go this way? Does it matter?

Replies from: chaosmosis, wedrifid, JonMcGuire
comment by chaosmosis · 2012-10-09T05:34:34.834Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

You'll quickly find that LessWrongers love tangents and digressions.

comment by wedrifid · 2012-10-09T10:10:58.095Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Or did EY mean for the discussion to go this way?

EY can mean whatever he wants. He gets to choose what is in the post, everyone else gets to choose what they would like to talk about.

comment by JonMcGuire · 2013-08-16T20:04:24.427Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

People focus on the motivations of the students and the professor because the professor's behavior is unorthodox. The students paid good money to learn about physics. As others have mentioned, you can't be too hard on them, they arrive at class expecting a physics lesson, not sleight-of-hand. Consequently, my initial response to the article was that I understood what EY meant to convey, but I thought there were probably other ways to illustrate it that didn't involve the unnecessary "trickery" demonstrated by the professor.

However, upon further reflection, the professor's trickery itself could be characterized as relevant to EY's point. If we completely ignore the proferred "magic explanations" from the students, one might consider the professor's trick a lesson that all the physics education in the world may be inadequate to explain a puzzling observation. In other words, I found it helpful to assume that the professor was also trying to make a point similar to that which EY was making, instead of assuming that the professor just felt like being a jerk that day.

As a bonus, by focusing on the conditions of the scenario instead of just the answers, a student who is smart enough to recognize that their education may be inadequate could still answer "I don't have enough information to explain this," which implies he still believes there is an explanation, which might be a better answer than just "I don't know," which sounds a lot like just giving up.

comment by jafrumstra · 2013-03-01T14:14:13.412Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

"Because of heat conduction" is the correct answer-heat from the radiator conducts to the plate-heat from the plate conducts to the air regardless of which side is being examined. The teacher asked "Why does this happen?" not "Why is the closest side to the radiator cold and the distant side hot?"-the question which is assumed to be implied by the actual one. The answer to THAT question would be "because of magic" since the professor was performing an illusion that was prepared ahead of time. The data points necessary to divine the nature of the illusion (Mr. Wizard) would be: temp of the radiator, average air temp, temp of the cold side, temp of the warm side...that would be sufficient to demonstrate that "because of heat conduction" one side was closer to the radiator while the other was exposed to the air-despite the appearance of the opposite circumstances. Fake explanation FTW

comment by RogerS · 2013-04-03T22:21:08.377Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Many different things can be deduced from this story, as previous comments have illustrated. The step that I question is "carries no information" = "magic". I prefer Karl Popper's account, in which [to paraphrase "Conjectures & Refutations" Chapter 1] "carries no predictive information" = "metaphysical" but "metaphysical" does not mean "unscientific". Rather, science involves two activities, hypothesis creation and hypothesis testing. It is the hypothesis testing that has to be exclusively empirical (confined to falsifiable hypotheses). There are no rules for arriving at new hypotheses according to Popper, only heuristics, and metaphysical arguments can often be a source of new insights that lead to new falsifiable hypotheses. I believe Imre Lakatos developed this distinction with his idea of "Research programmes" which cannot be falsified but get abandoned when they cease to be fruitful of falsifiable hypotheses. The commenters who have stressed that some of the student's wrong answers could be valuable first steps towards understanding fit into Popper's scheme. The question (which we can't answer) is whether the "password" status or the "first step" status was uppermost in their minds. To conclude, the posting is valuable in drawing attention to the disutility of password-type answers, but misleading in not also recognizing the role of first-step-type answers.

comment by A1987dM (army1987) · 2013-08-10T12:36:24.437Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

If you say "heat conduction", what experience does that lead you to anticipate? Under normal circumstances, it leads you to anticipate that, if you put your hand on the side of the plate near the radiator, that side will feel warmer than the opposite side.

Metals conduct heat much faster than air, so if conduction was the only mechanism responsible you'd expect both sides of the plate to be at nearly the same temperature.
comment by Mati_Roy (MathieuRoy) · 2013-09-14T21:36:51.254Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Would you consider "black energy" as a fake explanation for the expansion of the universe?

comment by Jiro · 2014-08-18T19:29:50.196Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

(Response to old post)

These are students, so they don't have perfect understanding of science. Even if they understand how to calculate what some theories predict, they don't know exactly when to apply those theories or what confounding effects might occur.

So unlike someone with perfect understanding, they don't know with 100% certainty that any specific theory applies. Asking what caused X to happen is really asking "what theory, among the ones you know, has the highest probability of having caused this result".

But even if the result is wrong and no theory actually would lead to that result, the students would grant some non-zero probability that each theory produced that result (since they know they imperfectly understand the theories). There would still be a highest probability. They would then say "conduction" and they would be correct--the probability, given their limited understanding, that conduction produced this result is non-zero and greater than the probability for, say, convection.

comment by Slvador · 2014-08-28T14:06:13.033Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

I have seen this example before. I actually do not blame the students at all for the following reasons (some taken from other comments)

1) They are thinking out loud, so seeing that some aspects points it could be heat conduction(after all that would be the typical reason for most temperature discrepancies withing an item) then they scream "heat conduction" as an invitation for closer look which is a valid (as pointed by other commentors) method of thinking

2) They are screaming the highest probable answer they can think of. Magic and heat conduction are not the same in that case. The students know they do not know the answer for sure and they basically going for the most probable cause that exist in their mind database and isnt that how a theory is initially theorized? after initial stating it get questioned and removed or confirmed

3) This is a physics class. It is assumed you do not know the answer so guessing is a valid way to learn because imagine in a different situation when it was NOT a trick question and the answer was actually heat conduction. The student who GUESSED heat conduction will feel the joy of being right and probably will remember that lesson better for the future since he will link it to a happy specific incident in the class. I think the student aim for that feeling and it is a good thing because I believe it helps them actually learn [need memory learning references]

4) They know (assume) the teacher knows the correct answer, and thus guessing wrong answers is not bad since no one will take their word for it and instead wait for the final correct answer by the teacher. There is no harm at all to guess and try to rationale through all of your theories. Stating the topic of those theories is a valid first step since that's how we usually solve any physics problem (e.g. to calculate the speed of falling ball, you need to recognize which equation you should investigate, should it be conservation of energy or the acceleration equations. Stating the title then investigating till proven right or wrong is a correct way to address this problem. Part of solving a problem is recognizing the telltales that direct you to the right equation, this case had a lot of telltales for heat conduction)

5) While this relates less, but i do not blame the students for not figuring out the right answer because its probability is pretty low. If they never got trick questions from this teacher, it seems a pretty unreasonable thing to assume from a physics teacher during class.

6) I would guess most of the students who would say "i dont know" FAST without exploring those wrong theories will be the lazy and uninterested students. it is equivalent to giving up and shutting down your rationale.

I am pretty confident if this happened to me i will be one of the students who keep trying theories and discussing them with others. I learned a LOT of physics concepts through discussing ideas with bunch of students who none knew the right answer. Many times we were able to reach the right answer after several wrong ones. I think in this situation this what the students were doing and I approve of it [sadly my approval means nothing :D].

Replies from: Basil Marte
comment by Basil Marte · 2019-01-29T03:37:31.272Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

I've not seen anybody mention those students who said "strange metals in the plate" in particular, and I'd like to argue for them. Their answer was not a password (the teacher never mentioned it), and actually shows correct anticipation-controlling beliefs! That is to say, they noticed that the observed outcome is not what they would have predicted, and looked for some hypothesis that explained why the heat gradient is reversed. Working from the incorrect assumption that they are seeing a stationary state, they guessed a hidden means of transporting heat from the heater to the far end of the plate, e.g. an insulated internal layer. I might be overfitting on this few-word detail, but I think this answer is on average very early in search orderings of those with qualitative but correct visualization of heat conduction, and I don't see other equally simple reasons why the students would have said this.

Of course, I also agree with the rationalist point that this answer should still feel a bit forced, trigger a listing of assumptions, which on average hits "stationary state" very early for simple physics problems.

comment by Yosarian2 · 2016-07-08T20:39:17.887Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

I was just re-reading the sequences, and I have to say that as a teacher I really think you're misjudging what is happening here.

Much of learning, it seems, is building up a mental framework, starting from certain concepts and attaching new concepts to them so that they can easily be recalled later and so you can use the connections between concepts to develop your own thinking later..

From my point of view, it looks like the student (perhaps as long as a year ago) had successfully created a new concept node in their mind, "heat conduction". They had connected this node to the concepts of heat transfer and physics. And even though they likely hadn't activated this node at all in perhaps a year, they were able to take a specific example of something they saw in the real world, generalize it to something that might be related to the more general topic of heat transfer in physics, and create a hypothesis of heat conduction.

If you saw a machine learning algorithm that was able to do all that, you'd really be impressed! Something like Watson might be able to go from the concept of heat transfer to heat conduction, but it wouldn't be able to generalize from a specific example of heat transfer it saw in the real world.

Now, they might not yet have a lot of details attached to the "heat conduction" concept node in their head. But that's ok, that they can learn, that gives you something to build on as a teacher. If you teach it well and they can attach some details and images and maybe some math to the concept of "heat conduction" in the head, then hopefully next time they'll say "Maybe heat conduction? Hmmm, no, that doesn't work." which is even better. But there's more going on here then just "guessing a password"; this is part of what constructing a model of the world looks like while the process is only partly completed.

Replies from: Wes_W
comment by Wes_W · 2016-07-11T02:52:08.334Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

I'm not sure you've described a different mistake than Eliezer has?

Certainly, a student with a sufficiently incomplete understanding of heat conduction is going to have lots of lines of thought that terminate in question marks. The thesis of the post, as I read it, is that we want to be able to recognize when our thoughts terminate in question marks, rather than assuming we're doing something valid because our words sound like things the professor might say.

Replies from: Yosarian2
comment by Yosarian2 · 2016-07-11T12:47:06.475Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Yeah, that's fair, although it sounds like the student he's quoting did understand that.

I'm just saying that "guessing the teacher's password" isn't usually a fair way to view what's going in in cases like this. "Building up a concept map of connections between related concepts" is probably more accurate, and that really is a vital part of the learning process, it's not a bad thing at all.

comment by DreamFlasher · 2017-02-01T08:18:06.559Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
  • We can be swayed by the context we are operating in, thinking inside-the-box
  • Don’t use terms and explanations if you are not really sure about the concepts
comment by Pod · 2019-10-06T00:35:43.695Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

I can't find the quoted joke in scijokes.txt, can anyone help?

comment by MoritzG · 2019-11-20T10:28:09.288Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

When you are presented with a very unlikely outcome you have to accept it.

Had the teacher shown a dozen dice all showing the same number and asked how he did it, there would have been two answers:

2. You

comment by MoritzG · 2019-11-20T11:13:20.413Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Had the teacher presented a dozen of dice all showing the same number and asked how this could have happened they would have been wiser.
But the situation is similar. In pure theory this could happen naturally, in that case doubting it would be a case of gamblers fallacy or not knowing the Anthropic principle.

If you encounter the impossible you should check your assumptions, but to say that a human like entity has caused this outcome is dangerous.

comment by MichaelDickens · 2023-03-17T12:55:31.991Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Perhaps it's worth distinguishing between two types of "I don't know":

  1. I don't know because I haven't put any thought into it. (This is the type of "I don't know" that teachers rightly discourage.)
  2. I don't know because I have considered several hypotheses, and none of them explain my observations. (For example, my mental model of heat conduction predicts that the close side of the plate should be hotter, not the far side, so that explanation fails.)

Perhaps teachers should encourage students to replace "I don't know" with "my mental model predicts A, but I observe B", which communicates that the student is thinking correctly about the problem.

comment by titotal (lombertini) · 2023-09-12T11:15:02.377Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

Note that the nearer side feeling colder than the farther side is be completely possible.

 The key is that they didn’t check the temperatures of each side with a thermometer, but with their hands. And your hands don’t feel temperature directly, they feel heat conduction. If you have a cake and a baking tin that are the same temperature, the metal will feel hotter because it is more conductive.

If I wanted to achieve the effect described here without flipping trickery, I might make the side near the radiator out of a very nonconductive plastic (painted to look like metal), and the side further away out of a very thermally conductive metal. It seems entirely plausible to me that once everything is in thermal equilibrium, the plastic would feel cooler than the metal, despite having a higher temperature. Of course, this would depend on the actual temperatures, conductivities, etc involved. But in principle it seems completely possible. 

Suddenly the physics students don’t look so foolish. In fact, in this scenario, both the heat conduction guy and the “strange metals in the plate” guy are on the right track! (even the convective guy could have a point, if that's the primary method of heating the far side). They are acknowledging their confusion, while trying to suggest avenues of exploration that might reveal an aspect of the problem they didn’t think of. In this scenario, the  Rationalist who dismisses these answers is the real fool, acting as the curiosity stopper, just because they didn't think of how hands feel heat. (of course, the ideal reasoner would think of both "different metal" and "flipped plate" as hypotheses). 

Replies from: Firinn, sharmake-farah
comment by Firinn · 2023-09-12T22:39:35.749Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

....I'm a little mindblown by reading this, honestly, because I read 'Fake Explanations' when I was like eleven years old, and I really felt like it changed the way I thought and was extremely influential on me at that early point in my life, and I kept telling people this story, and also I never thought of this, and now I am strongly negatively updating against my own success at internalising the lessons here. 

I guess the lesson from this is that the correct answer isn't "it's really obvious that the instructor flipped the plate around and the students should have realised this as soon as they Noticed They Were Confused", but "when you encounter confusing information, you should feel comfortable remaining confused until you have actually spent some time generating more hypotheses and learning more information". The answer of "the plate was flipped around" is semi-obvious (it's often the first hypothesis generated by smart people when I recount this story to them) and we all... stopped thinking at that point, and patted ourselves on the back for being so rational? 

This feels a bit like it deserves to inspire a top-level post along the lines of "there is a second higher-level version of the Fake Explanations post, which points out that weird metals is a possible explanation, and if you laughed at the guy who was considering the possibility of the plate being made out of some sort of weird material and considered yourself superior for not being so stupid, then you should feel bad and go reread the stuff about motivated stopping". Or something. 

Replies from: SaidAchmiz
comment by Said Achmiz (SaidAchmiz) · 2023-09-12T23:20:30.243Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

I agree that “it’s really obvious that the instructor flipped the plate around and the students should have realised this as soon as they Noticed They Were Confused” is not the correct answer. However, I think that your suggested lesson isn’t quite right either—namely, the “until…” clause is superfluous.

Now, I can’t speak for physics students, at whatever level of physics education the students in the story were at… but for myself, I don’t think I could’ve generated the hypothesis outlined in the grandparent. (Or, perhaps more precisely, maybe I could’ve generated at least approximately that hypothesis—but only alongside a number of other hypotheses which would be physically implausible/inapplicable/etc.)

In other words: there is no way I could’ve solved the puzzle (without first learning much more physics, which presumably is outside the scope of the problem).

And this, in my experience, happens often. There is some phenomenon, and you don’t know the explanation for it; there is some mystery, and you don’t know the solution to it. And the rational conclusion is that you aren’t going to figure out the answer. You just don’t know. You can spend some large amount of time or effort learning and developing expertise in the relevant domain, certainly! But you’re not going to figure out the right answer by thinking about it, because the space of possibilities for what the answer could be includes any number of unknown unknowns: things that you aren’t aware of, and that you don’t know you’re not aware of.

Thus the rational response is to follow the wisdom of Homer [edit: actually it was Bart] Simpson: can’t win, don’t try. You don’t know, and you can’t figure it out, and that’s all there is to it. Either invest the considerable effort needed to research the subject matter in general and the problem in particular, or simply stop at “I don’t know”.

Replies from: Firinn
comment by Firinn · 2023-09-13T00:46:23.990Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

I'm not a physics student, but I absolutely feel I should have been able to generate more than one hypothesis here! I have definitely enjoyed watching science videos that talk about really cool ceramics that get used in building spacecraft, which can be glowing red-hot and nevertheless safe to touch because of how non-conductive they are. So it's not like I wasn't aware of the possibility that some materials have weird properties here. It's just that I generated a single hypothesis - the instructor flipped the plate around - and was super-satisfied with being correct.

And maybe I get Bayes points for being correct, since "the instructor flipped the plate around" is the right answer (assuming it's a real story) and "the instructor went to all the trouble of constructing a two-sided plate out of really weird materials purely in order to fuck with his physics students" is a wrong answer. But where I think I went wrong is feeling derisive towards the silly incorrect physics students who say things like "maybe something weird is happening with heat conduction?" and feeling superior to them because they were just guessing the teacher's password. When, actually, "something weird is going on relating to conduction" is a thought which could have led to me generating and considering more than one hypothesis. 

comment by Noosphere89 (sharmake-farah) · 2023-09-13T01:11:56.885Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

This seems like a You Are Not Measuring What You Think Are Measuring moment. Link below:

https://www.lesswrong.com/posts/9kNxhKWvixtKW5anS/you-are-not-measuring-what-you-think-you-are-measuring [LW · GW]

comment by Ernesto G. (ernesto-g) · 2023-09-25T20:41:33.209Z · LW(p) · GW(p)

There are a few key aspects to consider here:
1 The instructor is an authoritative figure. Students assume the question posed by the instructor is one with merit and that he isn't playing tricks on them.
2 This is a physics class, not a philosophy class? If it was the second one, students may be inclined to think outside the box. But nobody expects a physics teacher to be playing tricks on the experiments.
3 Students will always try to give their best guess if they're pressed for an answer.  "I don't know" is rarely an acceptable answer by any teacher.