Why Have Sentence Lengths Decreased?
post by Arjun Panickssery (arjun-panickssery) · 2025-04-03T17:50:29.962Z · LW · GW · 21 commentsContents
21 comments
“In the loveliest town of all, where the houses were white and high and the elms trees were green and higher than the houses, where the front yards were wide and pleasant and the back yards were bushy and worth finding out about, where the streets sloped down to the stream and the stream flowed quietly under the bridge, where the lawns ended in orchards and the orchards ended in fields and the fields ended in pastures and the pastures climbed the hill and disappeared over the top toward the wonderful wide sky, in this loveliest of all towns Stuart stopped to get a drink of sarsaparilla.”
— 107-word sentence from Stuart Little (1945)
Sentence lengths have declined. The average sentence length was 49 for Chaucer (died 1400), 50 for Spenser (died 1599), 42 for Austen (died 1817), 20 for Dickens (died 1870), 21 for Emerson (died 1882), 14 for D.H. Lawrence (died 1930), and 18 for Steinbeck (died 1968). J.K Rowling averaged 12 words per sentence (wps) writing the Harry Potter books 25 years ago.
So the decline predates television, the radio, and the telegraph—it’s been going on for centuries. The average sentence length in newspapers fell from 35wps to 20wps between 1700 and 2000. The presidential State of the Union address has gone from 40wps down to under 20wps, and the inaugural addresses had a similar decline. (From Jefferson through T. Roosevelt, the SOTU address was delivered to Congress without any speech, and print was the main way that inaugural addresses were consumed for most of their history.) Warren Buffett’s annual letter to shareholders dropped from 17.4wps to 13.4wps between 1974 and 2013.
SlateStarCodex’s ten recommended blog posts have 22wps. My own top 10 posts have 20wps. Even top medical journals have under 25wps. The FAA, the European Commission, and various legal institutions have style guides recommending to stay under 20wps. Skimming r/writing, it looks like people recommend 10-15wps for fiction (HPMOR has 15wps). It’s possible that sentence lengths will stop declining only when we hit a physical limit on how short sentences can reasonably become. The best-selling hardboiled novella The Postman Always Rings Twice (1934) has 11wps, while I saw one source claiming that Jurassic Park (1990) has only 9wps.
Several explanations present themselves for why sentence lengths have decreased. They aren’t mutually exclusive; it could be that all of them contributed.
- The average reader has gotten dumber and prefers shorter, simpler sentences.
- Longer sentences are more suitable for reading out loud, but shorter sentences are more suitable for reading silently.
- Shorter sentences are just better, i.e. they promote faster reading and better comprehension.
The reason the average reader could have been smarter in the past is because literacy used to be more limited.
Full literacy didn’t appear until the turn of the 20th century in England. America had an earlier rise in literacy and the vast majority of free men could read by the 1800s, though like England it took until the 1900s to reach full literacy. It does seem broadly true that sentence lengths are higher in areas with more advanced readers; Stuart Little, the 1945 children’s book quoted at the top, has 13wps, while scientific journals often have 25wps. On the other hand, sentence lengths continued to decline throughout the 1900s, well after we reached full literacy.
Another theory is that journalists inspired a terser style. The newspaper industry grew throughout the 19th century and they saved money when they used fewer words. Many great American writers like Twain, Whitman, Hemingway, and Steinbeck were journalists and influenced by newspaper style. There are whole grammatical structures like the appositive noun phrase (the part set off by commas in “Mr. Smith, a Manhattan accountant, said…”) that are associated with newspapers and clearly have brevity in mind.
Another theory has to do with a transition from reading aloud to reading silently. Reading texts aloud to a group continued as a social practice into the Victorian era, and illiterates would even pay to listen to readings of Dickens. Works written up to this period would have often been written with listeners in mind. An interesting 2008 paper discusses how Dickens in particular uses punctuation and other markers to help orators read his novels. But eventually it became most common to read silently and one consequence was that punctuation became standardized on syntactic (i.e. grammatical) rather than prosodic grounds. I’m not sure if it follows that sentence lengths would also go down. Spoken language is surprisingly complex and actually contains more subordinate clauses than professional/academic writing. For example, I found some transcripts of interviews from Brandon Sanderson—a popular fantasy author whose Stormlight Archive series averages only 9 words per sentence—and measured his extemporaneous speech at ~20 words per sentence (and that includes a bunch of short sentences like “Yeah” or “I don’t know”).
The simplest theory is just that shorter sentences reflect better writing. When you see those ratings of a text’s reading difficulty in terms of a 4th-grade reading level or 10th-grade reading level and so on, those ratings are based on the Flesh-Kincaid readability score, which is just a weighted sum of the text’s words-per-sentence and syllables-per-word measures. A decrease of one grade level in readability thus comes from ~10 additional words per sentence or ~0.11 additional syllables per word. Studies invariably show that sentences with fewer words are easier for readers to understand quickly.
Others have suggested this for a long time; in one of the earliest analyses of sentence length, Lucius Sherman in Analytics of Literature (1893) wrote that the “heaviness” of sentences also decreased over time as sentence lengths decreased, and that “Elizabeth writers “are prevailingly either crabbed or heavy … ordinary modern prose, on the other hand, is clear, and almost as effective to the understanding as oral speech.”
Part of this was because older writers affected a Latinate style. The “periodic sentence,” which saves the main clause for the end after multiple dependent clauses are presented first, was common and exemplified in the extreme by writers like Samuel Johnson and Henry James. Consider the Stuart Little quote at the top: the main clause “Stuart stopped to get a drink of sarsaparilla” is preceded by a prepositional phrase “in the loveliest town of all” and four lengthy dependent clauses starting with “where.” This Latinate style included a preference for hypotaxis (connecting clauses with conjunctions or relative pronouns) over parataxis (presenting clauses sequentially without subordination):
Hypotaxis: When the alarm sounded, the firefighters, who had been sleeping, quickly jumped into action.
Parataxis: The alarm sounded. The firefighters had been sleeping. They quickly jumped into action.
It seems like the improved-readability effect provides most of the explanation. As more readers appeared and read more often (and read silently), selective pressure increased for styles that could be read and understood quickly. The telegraph and newspapers encouraged brevity as well. In principle, you could imagine that the Internet would have enabled a wordier style because it removed the financial costs of physically printing more words, but any effect like that hasn’t overcome the other trends.
21 comments
Comments sorted by top scores.
comment by David Gross (David_Gross) · 2025-04-04T00:34:33.207Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
There is a relatively new, practical reason to write short sentences: they are less likely to be mangled by automated translation software. Sentences often become long via multiple clauses. Automated translators can mangle such sentences by (for example) mistakenly applying words to the incorrect clause. If you split such sentences, you make such translations more reliable. Most of our writing now potentially has global reach. So you can be understood by more people if you meet translation software half-way.
comment by TAG · 2025-04-03T21:23:30.405Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
What is a sentence anyway... is there something special about a period, as opposed to other punctuation marks? Many are available: the colon is a possibility; also its half-brother; and the comma,of course...also the ellipsis -- even the mighty m-dash!
Replies from: mateusz-baginski↑ comment by Mateusz Bagiński (mateusz-baginski) · 2025-04-04T06:04:59.005Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
Since this is about written English text (or maybe more broadly, text in Western languages written in Latinic or Cyrillic), the criterion is: ends with a dot, starts with an uppercase letter.
comment by Kaj_Sotala · 2025-04-03T19:51:02.263Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
At first, I thought this post would be about prison sentences.
I got curious and checked if DeepResearch would have anything to add. It agreed with your post and largely outlined the same categories (plus a few that you didn't cover because you were focused on an earlier time than the screen era): "Cognitive Load & Comprehension, Mass Literacy & Broad Audiences, Journalism & Telegraphic Brevity, Attention Span & Media Competition, Digital Communication & Screen Reading, Educational & Stylistic Norms".
The last one I thought was interesting and not obvious from your post:
- Widespread literacy also had an effect on social norms. It wasn't just that sentences got shorter to accommodate the average reader, but also that it became more socially expected that writers accommodate the reader rather than the reader being expected to live up to the elite demands. This was partially connected to the rise of compulsory schooling. Once you're demanding that everyone learn to read, you kind of have to accommodate the limits of their abilities rather than just telling them "get good or gtfo".
- DR: More people could read, but to reach this broader audience, authors were compelled to write in a plainer style than the ornate constructions of previous centuries. We can view this as a shift in the social contract of writing: instead of readers straining to meet the text, the text was adjusted to meet the readers. Shorter sentences were a key part of that adjustment. [...] By the early 20th century, the norm had shifted – long-winded sentences were increasingly seen as bad style or poor communication, out of step with a society that valued accessibility.
- (This claim seems like it matches common sense, though DR didn't give me a cite for this specific bit so I'm unsure what it's based on.)
- DR also claimed that there was a "Plain Language movement" in the 1960s and 1970s, that among other things pushed for simpler sentences. Its only cite was to a blog article on readability.com, though Wikipedia also talks about it. You mentioned e.g. the Flesh-Kincaid formula in a descriptive sense, but it's also prescriptive: once these kinds of formulas get popularized as respected measures of readability, it stands to reason that their existence would also drive sentence lengths down.
- E.g. Wikipedia mentions that Pennsylvania was the first U.S. state to require that automobile insurance policies be written at no higher than a ninth-grade level (14–15 years of age) of reading difficulty, as measured by the F–K formula. This is now a common requirement in many other states and for other legal documents such as insurance policies.
There were a few other claims that seemed interesting at first but then turned to be hallucinated. Caveat deep researchor.
Replies from: Richard_Kennaway↑ comment by Richard_Kennaway · 2025-04-03T21:38:50.979Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
Re plain language movements, in the UK there were Gowers' "Plain Words" books from around that time (link provides links to full texts). I read these a very long time ago, but I don't recall if he spoke of sentence length, being mainly occupied with the choice of words.
comment by Mateusz Bagiński (mateusz-baginski) · 2025-04-03T18:33:52.967Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
Related: https://www.lesswrong.com/posts/Pweg9xpKknkNwN8Fx/have-attention-spans-been-declining [LW · GW]
Another related thing is that the grammar of languages appears to be getting simpler with time. Compare the grammar of Latin to that of modern French or Spanish. Or maybe not quite simpler but more structured/regular/principled, as something like the latter has been reproduced experimentally https://royalsocietypublishing.org/doi/10.1098/rspb.2019.1262 (to the extent that this paper's findings generalize to natural language evolution).
Replies from: simon, TAG↑ comment by simon · 2025-04-03T18:53:43.254Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
FWIW there is a theory that there is a cycle of language change, though it seems maybe there is not a lot of evidence for the isolating -> agglutinating step. IIRC the idea is something like that if you have a "simple" (isolating) language that uses helper words instead of morphology eventually those words can lose their independent meaning and get smushed together with the word they are modifying.
↑ comment by TAG · 2025-04-03T20:55:01.582Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
The idea that grammar is just inflection is misleading: languages that are mostly isolating can have complex ordering rules,like the the notorious adjective ordering of English.
As for french ...Moi, je ne me défile pas.
1st person. Sing.
1st person. Sing, again.
Negative.
1st person. Sing, reflexive.
Verb!!!
Negative,again.
Replies from: mateusz-baginski↑ comment by Mateusz Bagiński (mateusz-baginski) · 2025-04-04T06:02:38.803Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
Fair enough. Modify my claim to "languages tend to move from fusional to analytic (or something like that) as their number of users expands".
comment by DirectedEvolution (AllAmericanBreakfast) · 2025-04-04T01:38:49.286Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
Interestingly, breaking up long sentences into shorter ones by replacing a transitional word with a period does not quite capture the same nuance as the original. Here's a translation of Boccaccio, and a version where I add a period in the middle.
Wherefore, as it falls to me to lead the way in this your enterprise of storytelling, I intend to begin with one of His wondrous works, that, by hearing thereof, our hopes in Him, in whom is no change, may be established, and His name be by us forever lauded.
Wherefore, as it falls to me to lead the way in this your enterprise of storytelling, I intend to begin with one of His wondrous works. By hearing thereof, our hopes in Him, in whom is no change, may be established, and His name be by us forever lauded.
By replacing ", that," with a period, my revision completely changes our relationship with the narrator. In the original translation, the narrator is both announcing his goal and describing what he plans to do to achieve it.
In the revised version, he's describing his plan of action and a potential effect of that plan. We might assume that he's choosing that plan in order to bring about that effect, but it's no longer explicit in the text. Each sentence stands on its own. It's up to the reader to perceive the narrator's intention.
I wonder if inserting periods systematically tends to disrupt explicit links between intention and action. If so, perhaps the shortening of sentences reflects the anomie of the modern era, the gradual decay of an explicit moral framework in the stories we tell.
comment by Wei Dai (Wei_Dai) · 2025-04-04T05:32:07.928Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
In China, there was a parallel, but more abrupt change from Classical Chinese writing (very terse and literary), to vernacular writing (similar to speaking language and easier to understand). I attribute this to Classical Chinese being better for signaling intelligence [LW(p) · GW(p)], vernacular Chinese being better for practical communications, higher usefulness/demand for practical communications, and new alternative avenues for intelligence signaling (e.g., math, science). These shifts also seem to be an additional explanation for decreasing sentence lengths in English.
comment by DirectedEvolution (AllAmericanBreakfast) · 2025-04-04T00:49:26.182Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
Many short sentences can add up to a very long text. The cost of paper, ink, typesetting and distribution would incentivize using fewer letters, but not shorter sentences.
comment by Mitchell_Porter · 2025-04-04T03:15:58.548Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
Humans didn't always speak in 50-word sentences. If you want to figure out how we came to be trending away from that, you should try to figure out how, when, and why that became normal in the first place.
comment by jmh · 2025-04-04T14:01:43.806Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
Literacy seems to make sense to me but I might be missing something in the post. Writing is language and language is communication so at least two sides.
As more people learned to read, they also learned to write, and written communications increases. However, even with modest literacy one can read a long sentence. Or can do that when it is written by a good/skilled writer. But being able to read does not really lead to writing skills in most cases I suspect.
As more people started communicating via writing (think things like schools and education expansion) the skill level of the average writer likely declined. That probably lead to training next generation writes to write in a more simple sentence structure.
comment by cdt (nc) · 2025-04-03T20:52:23.582Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
This may be because editing has become easier and faster to iterate.
It's comparatively easy to identify sentences that are too long. Is it easy to identify sentences that are too short? You can always add an additional sentence, but finding examples where sentences themselves should be longer is much harder. With more editing cycles, this leads to shorter and shorter sentences.
comment by ProgramCrafter (programcrafter) · 2025-04-03T19:49:17.604Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
I suggest additional explanation.
The bigger the audience is, the more people there are who won't know a specific idea/concept/word (xkcd's comic #1053 "Ten Thousand" captures this quite succinctly), so you'll simply have to shorten.
I took logarithm of sentence length and linearly fitted it against logarithm of world population (that shouldn't really be precise since authors presumably mostly cared about their society, but that would be more time-expensive to check).
Relevant lines of Python REPL
>>> import math
>>> wps = [49, 50, 42, 20, 21, 14, 18, 12]
>>> pop = [600e6, 700e6, 1e9, 1.4e9, 1.5e9, 2.3e9, 3.5e9, 6e9]
>>> [math.log(w) for w in wps]
[3.8918202981106265, 3.912023005428146, 3.7376696182833684, 2.995732273553991, 3.044522437723423, 2.6390573296152584, 2.8903717578961645, 2.4849066497880004]
>>> [math.log(p) for p in pop]
[20.21244021318042, 20.36659089300768, 20.72326583694641, 21.059738073567623, 21.128730945054574, 21.556174959881517, 21.97602880544178, 22.515025306174465]
>>> 22.51-20.21
2.3000000000000007
>>> 3.89-2.48
1.4100000000000001
>>> 2.3/1.41
1.6312056737588652
>>> [round(math.exp(26.41 - math.log(w)*1.63)/1e9, 3) for w,p in zip(wps,pop)] # predicted population, billion
[0.518, 0.502, 0.667, 2.234, 2.063, 3.995, 2.652, 5.136]
>>> [round(math.exp(26.41 - math.log(w)*1.63)/1e9 - p/1e9, 3) for w,p in zip(wps,pop)] # prediction off by, billion
[-0.082, -0.198, -0.333, 0.834, 0.563, 1.695, -0.848, -0.864]
↑ comment by Kaj_Sotala · 2025-04-03T21:54:55.575Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
Wouldn't people not knowing specific words or ideas be equally compatible with "you can't refer to the concept with a single word so you have to explain it, leading to longer sentences"?
comment by leogao · 2025-04-03T22:22:56.331Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
shorter sentences are better because they communicate more clearly. i used to speak in much longer and more abstract sentences, which made it harder to understand me. i think using shorter and clearer sentences has been obviously net positive for me. it even makes my thinking clearer, because you need to really deeply understand something to explain it simply.
Replies from: arjun-panickssery↑ comment by Arjun Panickssery (arjun-panickssery) · 2025-04-03T22:27:25.758Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
Shorter sentences are better. Why? Because they communicate clearly. I used to speak in long sentences. And they were abstract. Thus I was hard to understand. Now I use short sentences. Clear sentences.
It's been net-positive. It even makes my thinking clearer. Why? Because you need to deeply understand something to explain it simply.
Replies from: leogao↑ comment by leogao · 2025-04-03T22:43:48.769Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
goodhart
Replies from: TsviBT↑ comment by TsviBT · 2025-04-04T05:52:18.095Z · LW(p) · GW(p)
Yes, and this also applies to your version! For difficult or subtle thoughts, short sentences have to come strictly after the long sentences. If you're having enough such thoughts, it doesn't make sense to restrict long sentences out of communication channels; how else are you supposed to have the thoughts?